Integrity rules

General rules of conduct for people, institutes and businesses that, for some unfathomable reason, happen to value integrity, and want to get rid of the culture of (self)censorship:

Definition

  • Integrity means being open and honest always.

Ground rules

  • We say the truth, the whole truth and nothing but the truth (as we see it). Grasping the truth is necessary for informed moral and political decision making.
  • We frequently ask ourselves: “Can we be open and honest about this?

If the answer is ‘No‘, we need a valid reason. A valid reason differs from a fallacy by holding up when challenged by the question ‘Really?’. Fallacies include the promotion of self-interest and avoiding retaliation based on fear.

If the answer is ‘Yes’, then we say ‘OK, let’s do it’, implying we will actually be open and honest about it.

  • A confidentiality agreement is a valid reason for not being open. Confidential information is kept confidential.

Regarding ourselves

  • We will no longer just say and think we are open and honest. We are critical about the level of integrity of ourselves and others until it shows, i.e. integrity is not just what we say. It is what we do.
  • We never think we can’t be wrong.
  • We never hide or twist the truth because of how others may respond to us being open and honest.
  • We say sorry if we were wrong & appreciate being corrected.

Regarding others

  • We are not paternalistic. Everybody has a right to decide for him/her self.
  • We are not only open and honest about things that matter from our point of view. We also show integrity regarding others may find relevant from their point of view.
  • We don’t take advantage in any way of people who are open and honest (from their own point of view). They get the benefit of the doubt, because integrity makes a person vulnerable enough in and of itself.
  • Whoever is right, can get the acknowledgement for being right.
  • Anyone who harms others solely because they have been open and honest is exposed to public disapproval of this harmful behaviour (just by being open and honest about it).

Regarding (social) space and time

  • Everyone is free to participate in societal debate provided they show integrity.
  • Employees also have freedom of expression in societal debate provided they make clear they are communicating a titre personnel, rather than as (an employee/representative of) a business or institute.
  • We are not only open and honest now. Integrity also applies to (being truthful about and taking responsibility for our actions in) the past.

Regarding businesses and institutes (including universities)

  • Businesses and institutes demand & cultivate open and honest internal and external communication.
  • They provide a safe environment to be open and honest.
  • They are resistant to complaints about employees communicating a titre personnel, and they are completely open and honest about such complaints, not only internally but also externally.
  • They have appropriate, operational and effective procedures to settle differences of opinion, and they are open and honest about cases, motivation (reasons) and outcomes regarding challenges involving integrity.
  • They have a zero-tolerance policy for improper arguments (e.g. fear) to waive a complaint/review procedure.

Why would we?

These integrity rules are necessary and sufficient conditions for a just and sustainable future.

Science & society, including in particular livestock farming, should be critical of her own integrity (so prove it rather than presuppose it), should have no fear of telling or knowing the truth, and should want to know, i.e. be ‘philo-sophos’, lover of wisdom & knowledge, much more so than being client-friendly or (bulk-)market-directed (see also this thread on Twitter).

Before you leave

I appreciated receiving open and honest feedback on what you think about these rules and how they could be improved.

Posted in Code of conduct, Ethics, Integrity, Justice, Justice, Politics, Science, Semantics | Tagged , | Leave a comment

Did European pig-welfare legislation reduce pig welfare? Perhaps not, but experts confirm that common indestructible materials are not proper enrichment for pigs at all, except perhaps for an enhanced novel branched-chains design.


Published as:
Marc B.M. Bracke and Paul Koene, 2019. Expert opinion on metal chains and other indestructible objects as proper enrichment for intensively-farmed pigs. PLOS ONE. Available at http://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0212610.

EC Directive 2001/93 requires that all pigs have access to proper investigation and manipulation materials. Intensively farmed pigs in Europe are frequently provided with a short/bare metal chain with or without an indestructible object attached to the chain. To date authorities are regarding this as proper enrichment. However, it has become increasingly clear that the chains do not provide proper enrichment, and that adding an indestructible object such as a ball, pipe or hard wood to the end of the chain may even reduce pig welfare. To test this hypothesis an expert survey was conducted. In total 36 international experts, mostly pig-welfare scientists, responded to the survey.

The experts only marginally agreed with the hypothesis (agreement score 4.6 on average on a scale from 0-10). However, indestructible materials generally received very low scores for welfare, indicating they did not provide proper enrichment. Ranked from low to high average welfare score, the objects were grouped in 5 significance levels:

Level 5 (totally insufficient): Chain hanging too high (for most of the smallest pigs in the pen; average score 1.3 on a scale from 0 to 10 where 5.5 would be ‘acceptable’)

Level 4 (extremely insufficient): Short chain (3.1), Small ball (2.8) and Big ball (average 2.5)

Level 3: (very insufficient) Pipe (3.5) and Bare chain (3.3)

Level 2-3 (very/rather insufficient): Hard wood (3.7)

Level 2 (rather insufficient): Chain on the floor (average: 4.4)

Level 1 (almost sufficient): Branched chains (5.1)

Compared to the marginal enrichment provided before the EC Directive 2001/93 was implemented in 2007 (in the Netherlands generally a short/bare chain, scoring 3.1 and 3.3 respectively, i.e. Level 3-4), adding balls or pipe , as commonly done in The Netherlands and Germany, does not improve pig welfare. Hard wood, as practised esp. in the UK, is a most marginal improvement (only 0.4 higher on average than Bare chain). Chain on the floor scored a bit better (4.4), without being acceptable (set at 5.5). The ‘new’ Branched chains scored significantly better than all other indestructible materials and its welfare score (5.1 on average) was close to the pre-defined level of acceptability (5.5 on a scale from 0, worst, to 10, best). The welfare benefits of adding balls, pipes or hard wood to the metal chain were marginal, and well below what the experts considered acceptable enrichment. The branched-chains design, by contrast, appears to be the most viable alternative. It involves providing a longer chain, i.e. with the free end reaching to floor level, adding ‘branches’, i.e. several short chains ending at the nose height of the pigs, and providing more chains per pen (i.e. 1 branched chain per 5 pigs). Therefore, the implementation of current pig-enrichment legislation needs revision. Branched chains should be implemented widely (across the globe) and in the short term as a first step towards, and benchmark for, providing proper enrichment to intensively-farmed pigs.

The above summary of the publication is a personal version of the first author. See also the related publication and posts on the FareWellDock website:

Chains as enrichment for pigs (Book chapter with supplement)
Pig animation – Improved, branched chain design as proper enrichment for pigs
Branched chains as enrichment for pigs (technical description, pictures and video)
Proper enrichment for intensively-farmed pigs – From review to preview
A collection of pictures of other enrichment materials for pigs can be found here: Prize contest (Prijsvraag) 2011.
Do pigs play with chains? Science versus society

Original abstract of the PLOS ONE paper:

Marc B.M. Bracke and Paul Koene, 2019. Expert opinion on metal chains and other indestructible objects as proper enrichment for intensively-farmed pigs. PLOS ONE. Available at http://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0212610.

Abstract

EC Directive 2001/93 requires that all pigs have access to proper investigation and manipulation materials. Intensively farmed pigs in Europe are frequently provided with a short metal chain with or without an indestructible object attached to the chain. To date authorities are regarding this as proper enrichment, perhaps with (in)direct reference to the RICHPIG model as a justification. However, it has become increasingly clear that the chains do not provide proper enrichment, and that adding an indestructible object to the end of the chain may even reduce rather than improve pig welfare. To test this hypothesis an expert survey was conducted containing 26 more or less compound questions. On a scale from 0 to 10 experts specified their level of agreement with the hypothesis, the prevalence and welfare scores of nine indestructible enrichment materials. In total 36 experts, mostly pig-welfare scientists, responded (response rate: 39%). Indestructible objects are less prevalent in countries that provide straw (like Sweden and the UK) and outside the EU (US). They are more prevalent in the Netherlands, Belgium, France and Finland, while the prevalence seems to be low in Spain. Balls, wood and pipes were provided most frequently: hard wood especially in the UK (as specified in farm assurance); indestructible balls and pipes in Germany and the Netherlands. The experts’ score for agreement with the hypothesis was only 4.6 on average (scale 0-10; n=25). Enrichment materials, ranked from high to low welfare score, were grouped in 5 significance levels as indicated by different superscripts based on Wilcoxon signed rank tests: Branched chains (5.1a), Chain on the floor (4.4b), Hard wood (3.7bc), Pipe (3.5c), Bare chain (3.3c), Short chain (3.1d), Small ball (2.8d), Big ball (2.5d), and Chain hanging too high (1.3e). Branched chains scored significantly better than all other indestructible materials and its welfare score (5.1 on average) was close to the pre-defined level of acceptability (5.5 on a scale from 0, worst, to 10, best). The welfare benefits of adding balls, pipes or hard wood to the metal chain were marginal, and well below what the experts considered acceptable enrichment. The branched-chains design, by contrast, appears to be the most viable alternative. It involves providing a longer chain, i.e. with the free end reaching to floor level, adding ‘branches’, i.e. several short chains ending at the nose height of the pigs, and providing more chains per pen (i.e. 1 branched chain per 5 pigs). Branched chains should be implemented widely and in the short term as a first step towards, and benchmark for, providing proper enrichment to intensively-farmed pigs.


Branched chain
Two organic pigs interacting simultaneously with a branched chain in the snow. Despite access to a straw bed for rooting, even organic pigs may interact with such chains for long periods of time, esp. directed towards the floor. In fact they will root the chain on the floor more than twice as much as playing with it in a horizontal position. In intensive pig production chains are often (too) short, and when a hockey-type ball or ‘sustainable’ plastic pipe is attached to the end of such a chain the pigs’ interest, and their welfare, is often even reduced further.
Two pigs playing simultaneously with a preferred anchor-type branched chain design.



Posted in Enrichment, Ethics, Experts, Future, Justice, Modelling, Pigs, Politics, Science | Tagged , , , , | Leave a comment

How can we feed the world?

How can we feed the world? That is no doubt a serious question. In this post I show why this question is also ambiguous in a way that may be blocking a sustainable solution.

The problem

The world population is growing, and thus demanding more food. However, agriculture is unlikely to keep up the pace, especially since poor nations are also expected to acquire lifestyles enjoyed in the West. To make things worse, current production methods are not sustainable due to issues with environmental degradation, climate change, and the impacts on public health and animal welfare. Since sustainable production is generally less productive, food shortages can be anticipated, potentially leading to hunger and social conflict. Thus, we really need to find a way to feed the world. But how?

Ambiguous

In order to pinpoint the ambiguities in this question, let’s break it down into its parts, and discuss each part in turn: How – can – we – feed – the world?

How

How – suggests a technological solution is called for. Indeed, it would be wonderful if we could redesign current systems, making them both more productive and sustainable at the same time. However, such a technological innovation would at best be a temporary solution, because main underlying causes are chronic and not to be found out there in the world.

Can

Can – suggests optimism about our problem-solving abilities. An impressively efficient food-production system has been developed since the Second World War. Smartphones and solar panels may further justify optimism about our technological capabilities. So, perhaps we can tackle the food issue as well. But the problem is complex, as our food system is built on a long series of technological innovations and it is extremely resistant to alternatives lacking an appealing business model. Our food system is also firmly embedded in fixed belief structures concerning free will in free markets, which, soon or later, will probably have to be changed as well. We have shown excellence in mastering the world through technological innovation. But I don’t think we can be as optimistic when it comes to mastering ourselves.

We

We – may refer to ‘all of us’, i.e. how can we feed the world together? But ‘we’ may also refer to an elite group of people, such as a consortium of research partners or the farmers of a small country. In this sense, ‘we’ may be portraying ourselves as having divine powers like dropping manna from the sky. But do ‘we’ really want to feed the world because we care for people as Jesus presumably did? Or are ‘we’ primarily raising the question as part of a business model, e.g. to raise money to open up new markets or to do (more) innovative research? Or, as Mark Bittman put it: “Feeding the world” might as well be a marketing slogan for Big Ag, a euphemism for “Let’s ramp up sales” (NYT, Oct 14, 2013).

Feed

Feed – suggests that we are like a mother that ‘feeds’ her children. Alternatively, it may mean that we are farmers selling produce, such that people can feed their families, if they can afford it. In fact, however, it more often means that ‘we’ will be working on innovations so (some) farmers can (just) stay in business. So, the word ‘feeding’ is not to be taken literally at all. In addition, the word ‘feeding’ suggests providing sufficient nutrients to sustain life. But the thing is, we are already producing enough food to feed the global population of 2050. Therefore, since more affordable food is likely to lead to an increase in demand, it may concern a quest for sustained decadence, rather than providing nutrients per se. As long as one third of our food is wasted, farmers are growing tulips rather than crops, and consumers are eating burgers rather than beans, we may well be dealing with a luxury problem here, rather than a basic human need.

The world

The world – suggests that we will be feeding all the living creatures of the world, including all people, plants and animals, wild and tame. However, ‘the world’ is more likely used here as a shorthand for ‘the world’s population of humans (only)’. In other words, in this question, we humans are the world. That, of course, is false. We are not the world. In fact, it’s more likely we have been destroying the world. And in the process we seem to have ‘forgotten’ that animals matter as well, and that we and all other animals need the natural environment in order to flourish. But, this seemingly innocent semantic mistake of equating us with the world is reason for concern. It may be indicative of exactly the frame of mind, characterised by oversimplifying economic rationalisation and cognitive dissonance, that has resulted in both the excellent production efficiency and the apparent unsustainability of our current food-production system. As long as that frame of mind remains ‘in business’ and we continue to ignore our own responsibilities, we have little chance of properly addressing the problem.

Conclusion

The question ‘How can we feed the world?’ poses a real challenge, but it does so in a most ambiguous way. In fact, every part of this question is raising one or more ambiguities. It is hard to see how this issue can be solved, if we remain over-optimistic about technological solutions, while largely ignorant about underlying mechanisms and our own moral responsibilities. It will be extremely difficult to change our habits and ‘re-invent’ ourselves. Therefore, we urgently have to start acknowledging that we ourselves are the root cause of the problem that needs to be tackled.

Sunset on the world
See also my related posts on Integrity and the circular welfare economy (and the related column in Dutch).

Posted in Animal welfare, Ethics, Food, Future, Happiness, Money, Politics, Population, Public, Science | Tagged , , , , , | Leave a comment

Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – 1. Introduction

This is post 1. “Introduction” of:

Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions

Marc B.M. Brackea, T. Bas Rodenburgb, Herman M. Vermeera, Thea G.C.M. van Niekerka
a Wageningen Livestock Research
b Wageningen University, Dept. of behavioural ecology

Reading guide

This is one of 8 blog posts under the heading of: “Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions”. It contains the following sections/posts:

  1. Introduction, specifying the need to compare feather pecking (fp) in layers and tail biting (tb) in pigs
  2. Terminology, specifying the various concepts involved in fp/tb.
  3. Overview of main similarities and differences between feather pecking and tail biting
  4. Farmer as a risk factor, emphasising, perhaps for the first time, that the farmer is a kind of ‘animal’ that is part of the problem (and the solution).
  5. Models, reviewing available conceptual models of fp and tb, as well as presenting a new ‘face model’.
  6. Disease framework, arguing that fp/tb may be regarded as a medical disorder, over and above being an abnormal behaviour per se.
  7. Evolution and domestication, emphasising the need to view fp/tb as a phenomenon an evolutionary and genetic background.
  8. References

The entire text (8 posts) can be downloaded as one pdf here.

1. Introduction

Feather pecking (fp) in poultry and tail biting (tb) in pigs are among the most persistent animal-welfare problems associated with intensive livestock farming. Both problems have been studied and reviewed extensively (e.g. fp: (Rodenburg et al., 2008; Nicol et al., 2013; Rodenburg et al., 2013); tb: (Schrøder-Petersen and Simonsen, 2001; Bracke et al., 2004a; EFSA, 2007b; Taylor et al., 2010; D’Eath et al., 2014; Valros, 2017)). Legislation and policy initiatives have been discouraging the continued performance of routine mutilations (beak treatment and tail docking for fp and tb respectively). However, both poultry and pig farmers generally find it difficult to stop mutilations and prevent and/or treat these injurious behaviours in intensive farming systems. Comparing fp and tb may help address these problems. However, few papers have compared the two forms of abnormal behaviour in detail. One notable exception is the fairly recent Open-Access publication by Brunberg et al. (2016). These authors discussed similarities and differences between fp and tb, and presented a general model which looks somewhat like an envelope. This publication is written for a scientific audience, and it is not easy to read for farmers and others interested in solving fp/tb such as vets, other farm advisors and NGOs. Also the ‘envelope-shaped’ model presented by Brunberg et al. (2016) is not as appealing as we would (ideally) like it to be. It mainly says that by nature both pigs and poultry are omnivorous generalists that have (had to) become production specialists via genetic selection and rearing in large-scale intensive systems applying a one-size-fits-all principle. According to Brunberg et al. both the physical and social environment (‘where you are’ and ‘who is with you’), together with animal-related factors (‘who you are’) determines ‘what you become’ in terms of fp or tb, i.e. a performer (pecker/biter), victim/receiver or a neutral animal. The authors also hypothesise that the gut-microbiota-brain axis may play a crucial role which should be investigated further. This is in accordance with the common view that fp and tb are multifactorial problems associated with the substantial discrepancy between the natural and the commercial environment resulting in a (seriously) deprived foraging (and/or feeding) motivation that eventually leads to fp/tb (and worse, i.e. cannibalism, if not curtailed adequately).

It is not entirely clear, however, why the model (figure) in Brunberg et al. (2016) should look like an envelope. When looking a bit more closely at the figure, the model appears to encompass everything (the animal, its history and its entire, physical and social, environment). Only upon more careful examination and in particular when reading the text itself do the further ramifications underlying the model become more clear. Since we feel the text may be rather inaccessible for practical application in problem solving, one objective of these blog posts, therefore, is to compare this model to other models, esp. those developed in our own organisation (Wageningen University & Research), in order to see if we can better highlight the available knowledge that should be used to (eventually help) solve the problem in practice. To this end we have also tried to make the information presented by Brunberg et al. (2016) more accessible, and we supplemented it with our personal expertise on fp/tb. It is important to emphasise, however, that the primary aim of this publication is to improve on the available conceptual frameworks to facilitate practical understanding of fp and tb so as to support solving the problem in the future. We do not, however, aim to present a tool box or cook book for solving fp/tb.

Reading guide

This was blog post nr. 1 under the heading of: “Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions”. It contains the following sections/posts:

  1. Introduction, specifying the need to compare feather pecking (fp) in layers and tail biting (tb) in pigs
  2. Terminology, specifying the various concepts involved in fp/tb.
  3. Overview of main similarities and differences between feather pecking and tail biting
  4. Farmer as a risk factor, emphasising, perhaps for the first time, that the farmer is a kind of ‘animal’ that is part of the problem (and the solution).
  5. Models, reviewing available conceptual models of fp and tb, as well as presenting a new ‘face model’.
  6. Disease framework, arguing that fp/tb may be regarded as a medical disorder, over and above being an abnormal behaviour per se.
  7. Evolution and domestication, emphasising the need to view fp/tb as a phenomenon an evolutionary and genetic background.
  8. References

The entire text (8 posts) can be downloaded as one pdf here.

Acknowledgements

These blog posts have been made possible by the Hennovation project (HORIZON 2020 ISIB-02-2014 project, Grant no. 652638).

Posted in Enrichment, Feather pecking, Laying hens | Tagged , , | Leave a comment

Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – 2. Terminology

This is post 2 on “Terminology” of:

Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions

Marc B.M. Brackea, T. Bas Rodenburgb, Herman M. Vermeera, Thea G.C.M. van Niekerka
a Wageningen Livestock Research
b Wageningen University, Dept. of behavioural ecology

Reading guide

This is one of 8 blog posts under the heading of: “Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions”. It contains the following sections/posts:

  1. Introduction, specifying the need to compare feather pecking (fp) in layers and tail biting (tb) in pigs
  2. Terminology, specifying the various concepts involved in fp/tb.
  3. Overview of main similarities and differences between feather pecking and tail biting
  4. Farmer as a risk factor, emphasising, perhaps for the first time, that the farmer is a kind of ‘animal’ that is part of the problem (and the solution).
  5. Models, reviewing available conceptual models of fp and tb, as well as presenting a new ‘face model’.
  6. Disease framework, arguing that fp/tb may be regarded as a medical disorder, over and above being an abnormal behaviour per se.
  7. Evolution and domestication, emphasising the need to view fp/tb as a phenomenon an evolutionary and genetic background.
  8. References

The entire text (8 posts) can be downloaded as one pdf here.

2. Terminology

In the next posts we will summarise similarities and differences between feather pecking (fp) in laying hens and tail biting (tb) in pigs, taking Brunberg et al (2016) as a starting point. We will also characterise the different models that have been proposed before on fp/tb. Building on this we will argue why we think that fp/tb may/should be regarded as a medical/mental disorder, provided the medical framework maintains an evolutionary and scientific perspective on fp/tb.

This post aims to characterise the underlying concepts and criteria, so as to illustrate that giving crisp definitions may not be as easy as it may seem to be at first sight.

Note: We will use the label ‘fp/tb’ in the remainder of these related posts to refer to the communal problem. It is difficult to provide an overarching term for fp and tb together. Most existing terms are too wide: Abnormal behaviour, injurious behaviour and harmful-social behaviour, e.g. because there are other forms of abnormal behaviour and because there are other forms of injurious behaviours like aggression (e.g. vulva-biting in sows) and abrasive behaviours (injuries resulting from making contact to flooring or pen fittings; cf fin injuries in farmed fish (Noble et al., 2012; Stien et al., 2013; Pettersen et al., 2014; Folkedal et al., 2016)).

An outbreak of injurious fp/tb requires a specification of the start and end point, i.c. presence of injuries. Here, again, the observer may play a significant role: the detection of injuries depends e.g. on the inspection frequency and quality (e.g. method & expertise) of the observer. The observer also plays a role in so-called early-detection and in decision-making as to when and what treatment is to be started to counteract an on-going outbreak.

It should also be emphasized that fp/tb is a process, where different types of animals are involved. In order to start, one ‘neutral’ animal must become an actor (pecker/biter) showing fp/tb behaviour towards a victim/receiver resulting in a fp/tb wound. When the outbreak escalates more and more individuals become involved and/or wounds become progressively severe, potentially leading to the death of the victim (such that the fp/tb may at some point be called ‘cannibalism’). Wounds may also get infected, thereby aggravating the impact on productivity and welfare. Some animals in a fp/tb pen may not get involved. These may be labelled ‘neutrals’. In addition, Brunberg et al. (2016) use the term ‘controls’ for animals in neighbouring pens which are not affected by fp/tb. These different types of individuals involves are not fixed over time. E.g. both neutrals and controls are labels that may changes over time (Daigle et al., 2015), i.e. animals that were neutrals/controls today, may become actors or victims tomorrow, and individuals may be both actor and victim at some point in time (or even at the same time). When an outbreak ends, both actors and victims may return to being ‘neutrals’, even though it is generally recognised that the probability of recurrence is much bigger in groups that have previously experienced fp/tb problems, as if the ‘set points’ of such animals have changed irreversibly. Because of this rather irreversible state-change it is important to differentiate between prevention, what is done to prevent an outbreak, and curative treatment, what is done to stop an outbreak that has occurred.

A final term used in these posts is the word ‘model’, by which we primarily mean a figure intended to explain fp/tb. Ideally, the model should not only illustrate the mechanism and the types of individuals involved, and where/how it goes wrong (e.g. that fp/tb is a multifactorial problem), but also provide answers to the other 3 why questions (evolution, function, ontogeny). Ideally, also the model should explain anomalies (i.e. apparently ‘strange’ facts) and generate testable predictions. The ideal model should also be effective in communicating what is the (e.g. welfare or production) problem associated with fp/tb and provide suggestions regarding prevention and/or treatment. Also, a model is better if it uses a stronger, more intuitively appealing metaphor, such that it is easily remembered, not only by scientists, but also by other stakeholders, i.c. farmers, their advisors, and NGOs (see e.g. cartoons at http://www.featherwel.org/). However, besides addressing all of these aspects, a good model should not be complex, but rather explain fp/tb in the most parsimonious way possible.

Reading guide

This was blog post nr. 2 under the heading of: “Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions”. It contains the following sections/posts:

  1. Introduction, specifying the need to compare feather pecking (fp) in layers and tail biting (tb) in pigs
  2. Terminology, specifying the various concepts involved in fp/tb.
  3. Overview of main similarities and differences between feather pecking and tail biting
  4. Farmer as a risk factor, emphasising, perhaps for the first time, that the farmer is a kind of ‘animal’ that is part of the problem (and the solution).
  5. Models, reviewing available conceptual models of fp and tb, as well as presenting a new ‘face model’.
  6. Disease framework, arguing that fp/tb may be regarded as a medical disorder, over and above being an abnormal behaviour per se.
  7. Evolution and domestication, emphasising the need to view fp/tb as a phenomenon an evolutionary and genetic background.
  8. References

The entire text (8 posts) can be downloaded as one pdf here.

Acknowledgements

These blog posts have been made possible by the Hennovation project (HORIZON 2020 ISIB-02-2014 project, Grant no. 652638).

Posted in Enrichment, Feather pecking, Laying hens | Tagged , , , , | Leave a comment

Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – 3. Overview

This is post 3 on “Overview of main similarities and differences between feather pecking and tail biting” of:

Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions

Marc B.M. Brackea, T. Bas Rodenburgb, Herman M. Vermeera, Thea G.C.M. van Niekerka
a Wageningen Livestock Research
b Wageningen University, Dept. of behavioural ecology

Reading guide

This is one of 8 blog posts under the heading of: “Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions”. It contains the following sections/posts:

  1. Introduction, specifying the need to compare feather pecking (fp) in layers and tail biting (tb) in pigs
  2. Terminology, specifying the various concepts involved in fp/tb.
  3. Overview of main similarities and differences between feather pecking and tail biting
  4. Farmer as a risk factor, emphasising, perhaps for the first time, that the farmer is a kind of ‘animal’ that is part of the problem (and the solution).
  5. Models, reviewing available conceptual models of fp and tb, as well as presenting a new ‘face model’.
  6. Disease framework, arguing that fp/tb may be regarded as a medical disorder, over and above being an abnormal behaviour per se.
  7. Evolution and domestication, emphasising the need to view fp/tb as a phenomenon an evolutionary and genetic background.
  8. References

The entire text (8 posts) can be downloaded as one pdf here.

3. Overview of main similarities and differences between feather pecking and tail biting

Table 1 shows an overview of similarities and differences between feather pecking (fp) in poultry (i.c. laying hens) and tail biting (tb) in pigs (i.c. weaned and growing/fattening pigs).

Table 1 is based primarily on Brunberg et al. (2016) and supplemented with our own (esp. MB and TvN) knowledge about fp and tb (also as presented on the henhub website www.henhub.eu). The table is intended to summarise the most relevant similarities and differences between fp (in hens) and tb (in pigs), and thus support decision making in dealing with fp/tb in practice.

The key risk in fp/tb is the fact that both laying hens and pigs are originally omnivorous generalists that have been become production specialists in feed intake and food conversion in intensive farming conditions. The motivation for fp/tb relates to a frustrated foraging need, which is modulated by a whole array of different risk factors, hence resulting in this multifactorial welfare issue. In addition to similarities the table also identifies a number of differences between fp in layers and tb in pigs, e.g. we don’t have genetically selected lines for tail biting comparable to the high and low fp lines in poultry. Hence, an experimental model to study tb in more detail is currently largely lacking (though pigs selected for social breeding value (high indirect genetic effects for growth) showed considerably less ear biting, tail damage, aggression and enrichment manipulation (Camerlink et al., 2015), and may thus in principle be suited to be used to study tb experimentally in more detail).

The table may perhaps be improved upon further by specifying relationships between the items specified as risk factors (in the left column) and the different responses identified in the process of fp/tb (in the right column; cf (Fraser, 1987a)).

Another suggestion relates to the many risk factors that may hamper practical problem solving. While scientific experiments necessarily vary only a few risk factors in order to reliably examine which factors may affect fp/tb, a tentative suggestion for solving the multifactorial fp/tb problem could be to try to formulate multifactorial solutions. This may be esp. relevant when monofactorial solutions fail to solve the problem. However, an important drawback of this approach is that it may essentially remain unclear which factors are accountable for any (positive or negative) results. When a multifactorial approach is working, it may be possible to tease out in subsequent research the relative contribution of the different risk factors. When it doesn’t work, that may be the end of the road for that particular type of farming (given the constraints imposed).

Table 1. Comparing risk factors and animal responses related to feather pecking (fp) in poultry (laying hens) and tail biting (tb) in pigs, taking Brunberg et al. (2016) as a starting point, supplemented with own author expertise (marked as *). Common risk factors (similar across species, specified between brackets when ‘unknown’) are followed by notable differences between Hens and Pigs (specified on the next lines). Black characters: risk increasing factors; green: risk decreasing factors (benefits); red (in the column ‘risk factors’): particularly welfare-reducing risk factors. ‘No’: means that the opposite reduced fp/tb.; behav.: behaviour; envir.: environment; decr.: decreased; incr.: increased; HFP: high fp line/breed; LFP: low fp line; TIM: tail-in-mouth; w: weeks; d: days; mo: months. The cells in column ‘Responses’ are not directly (horizontally) related to the risk factors. Responses are stacked: more positive behaviours are presented at the bottom; worst (form of escalation, i.e. cannibalism) is shown on top. Responses are related to ‘type of animal’ (victim, actor, neutral) (with welfare aspects specified at the level of the type-of-animal label). Poor welfare responses are shown in red (in the column ‘responses’). See the text for a more detailed description of how to read the table.

Type of factorRisk factors (Multifactorial, related to the type of factor, i.e. environment-, group- & animal based)Responses (behav., physiology, pathology & welfare, related to type of animal, i.e. victim, actor, neutral)Type of animal
Envir.-basedModern large-scale specialised farmsVictim: fear, pain (during outbreak), stress, sickness (during treatment, recovery)Victim
 Barren pen (no proper foraging material, straw), large discrepancy between intensive farming envir. and the natural envir./envir. of evolutionary adaptationCannibalism 
 (Partly) slatted floor
Hens: (Litter)
Pigs: Concrete
(Wound) infection 
 Indoors*
Hens: Range (may provide foraging opportunities and reduce stocking density)
Pigs: – (Outdoor area may provide rooting substrate (soil), fibre (pasture), but not necessarily)
Production loss (reduced growth)
Hens: Egg laying (reproduction)
Pigs: Growth (production)
 
 One size fits all (food, climate*)  
 Standardised feed, optimised for average individual (vs indiv. needs); perhaps probiotics may treat fp/tb
Hens: –
Pigs: No phase feeding; decr. feeding frequency predicted tb outbreaks 9 w later; tb victims made more feeder visits 2-5w prior to tb
Appearance
Hens: Deteriorating feather cover
Pigs: Tucked tails
 
 Feed changes and ‘hiccups’ in providing feed (unpredictable frustration)Decreased tryptophan, serotonin levels 
 Feed type; Reduced feeding time, not ground, concentrated feed, less fibre
Hens: Pellets give more fp than fine ground feed; no mash/pecking materials; high E diet; no feathers in diet (acting as fibre, incr. feed passage)
Pigs: Contradictory results (liquid/pellets/meal) but straw reduces tb & is consumed
(Fp/tb) Wound(s)
Hens: Esp. tail, body (not back of head)
Pigs: Tail (possibly ears, flanks, legs)
 
 Protein, mineral (NaCl) deficiency; supra-nutritional NaCl may alleviate fp/tb
Hens: Deficiency of crude protein, amino acids, minerals (Na, Ca)
Pigs: Nutritional imbalance incr. tb
  
 Feeder space, feed competition (bite/peck to get access to feed)Salivation (pH incr.; alleviate peptic ulcers) 
 Rearing conditions (both poor rearing conditions and a backdrop from enriched rearing conditions to deprived conditions later in life)
Hens: Absence of litter around 5w, high stocking densities, rearing on wire floor
Pigs: More piglets/stockperson, fostering, no straw in farrowing pen, reduced feeder space during rearing gives more tb later in life; multi-litter rearing decr. manipulative behav.; providing straw during rearing and then depriving pigs of straw later is also considered a risk factor
[Microbiota composition?]
Hens: HFP has different microbiota composition than LFP; feather eating changes gut microbiota;
Pigs: Unknown
 
 (Pen size, pen design)
Hens: (Large)
Pigs: (Small)
Escalation of tp/tb (outbreak) 
Group-based (envir.- & animal based)Group housing
Hens: (Very) large groups (10-100.000 birds)
Pigs: Small (~10 pigs)
Arousal, restlessness, excitement (positive), fear & avoidance (negative).
Hens: Cut feathers increased fp
Pigs: Blood tail model (rope) increased (tail) biting behav.
 
 High stocking density
Hens: More fp in largest groups (15-120 birds)
Pigs: (Not uniform results)
Cognition, (social) learning, (synchronisation; copy-behaviour; stimulus enhancement) 
 Farm health status (any (major) stressor/immune suppressor probably)
Hens: Vaccination (specific immune stimulation) when young may incr. fp as adults; LFP have better immunocompetence; e.g. E. Coli incr. severe fp
Pigs: Better health status reduces tb; straw reduces infections
Prevalence/intensity:
Hens: Fp on 86% of UK flocks; SFP esp. when adult; fp up to 135 bouts/bird/hr; 3 severe pecks/min
Pigs: Tb on 30-70% of farms; fanatic biters bite 11-25% of time
 
 Mutilation (3 aspects are relevant: 1. Method used; 2. Amount of tissue removed; 3. Age of treatment; esp. 2nd aspects is relevant as risk factor)   Hens: Beak treatment (previous beak trimming (may remove larger/smaller part of the beak), now infrared beak treatment) (Note: In poultry, as it were the (future) actor is mutilated)Severe fp (SEP)/tb   Hens: – Pigs: Three types of (severe/injurious) tb: two-stage (starting with TIM), sudden forceful, and obsessive (fanatic) 
 Pigs: Tail docking (longer or shorter part of the tail) (Note: in pigs as it were the (future) receiver is mutilated by removing the tail)Actor: (Excitement, pleasure [during outbreak], pain, stress [during treatment])
Hens: Pecker
Pigs: Biter
Actor, performer
 History of fp/tb (once an outbreak has occurred, the likelihood of another outbreak increases; animals are never the same again after an outbreak; (irreversibly) changed set points)Object-direction:
Hens: Towards feathers
Pigs: Towards the tail
 
Animal-based(Bred for) very high production-efficiency (genetics, breeds) (esp. genetic motivation of feed-related behav.; behavioural need to species-specific foraging behav); fp/tb has moderate heritability (~0.2)Neutral (in same pen)/control (on other pen):
Neutral as a biter in spe: boredom, frustration, behavioural deprivation, esp. of foraging motivation
Neutral / control
 Hens: (Eggs)
Pigs: Lean meat; neutrals have different genetics
Gentle manipulation
Hens: Gentle fp is prevalent in young birds, decr. with age
Pigs: Tail in mouth (TIM),
 
 Domesticated 5-6000 years ago; bred in 50 years of intensive selection from foraging generalists (omnivorous (variable diet; need to explore)) to meat & egg producing specialists; fp/tb not selected against; tb&fp are correlated to production, but not in the same wayPen-mate directed exploration
Hens: (Deteriorated) plumage condition
Pigs: Wet tails
 
 Hens: Male peckers had higher body fat; female peckers had earlier onset of lay; HFP: Better growth, lower total egg mass, decr. feed efficiency
Pigs: Lower backfat, lean tissue growth
Consummatory behav.:
Hens: Feather eating (more in HFP)
Pigs: –
 
 Being different
Hens: Plumage colour (standing out from others; incidental pigmented birds were more often victims)
Pigs: [Lame pigs get bitten]
Object/substrate-directed exploration/foraging in accordance with nature, showing natural behav. (50-60% of time)
Hens: Scratching, pecking
Pigs: Rooting, biting
 
 Personality
Hens: Peckers appear more proactive, fearful (in open field), stress (cortisol shows variable results); more foraging & walking when young incr. fp as adults; HFP more active; mobility to get to the nestboxes (i.e. too calm birds are at risk for fp)
Pigs: Low backtest responders showed less pen-mate manipulation; biters more sitting & kneeling 6d prior to tb; victims more posture changes 6 d prior to tb; tail posture (tucked) may predict tb 2-3d before outbreak
  
 Sex, probably females more active performers
Hens: All females
Pigs: Mixed/uni-sex; males receive more tb; uncastrated males are more likely to become fanatic biters (1 study)
  
 Age: Onset around sexual maturity (also then shifting nutritional needs)
Hens: Adult (16-80wks); progesterone (& oestrogen) incr. up to 18w incr. fp; testosterone decr. fp; SFP ~20w in females, but not males
Pigs: Young, prepubertal (<5-6mo); perhaps associated with teething*
  
 Body weight
Hens: –
Pigs: Biters are lighter; victims tend to be heavier before tb (later decr. growth)
  

Reading guide

This was blog post nr. 3 under the heading of: “Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions”. It contains the following sections/posts:

  1. Introduction, specifying the need to compare feather pecking (fp) in layers and tail biting (tb) in pigs
  2. Terminology, specifying the various concepts involved in fp/tb.
  3. Overview of main similarities and differences between feather pecking and tail biting
  4. Farmer as a risk factor, emphasising, perhaps for the first time, that the farmer is a kind of ‘animal’ that is part of the problem (and the solution).
  5. Models, reviewing available conceptual models of fp and tb, as well as presenting a new ‘face model’.
  6. Disease framework, arguing that fp/tb may be regarded as a medical disorder, over and above being an abnormal behaviour per se.
  7. Evolution and domestication, emphasising the need to view fp/tb as a phenomenon an evolutionary and genetic background.
  8. References

The entire text (8 posts) can be downloaded as one pdf here.

Acknowledgements

These blog posts have been made possible by the Hennovation project (HORIZON 2020 ISIB-02-2014 project, Grant no. 652638).

Posted in Enrichment, Feather pecking, Laying hens | Tagged , , , | Leave a comment

Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – 4. Farmer as a risk factor

This is post 4 on “Farmer as a risk factor” of:

Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions

Marc B.M. Brackea, T. Bas Rodenburgb, Herman M. Vermeera, Thea G.C.M. van Niekerka
a Wageningen Livestock Research
b Wageningen University, Dept. of behavioural ecology

Reading guide

This is one of 8 blog posts under the heading of: “Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions”. It contains the following sections/posts:

  1. Introduction, specifying the need to compare feather pecking (fp) in layers and tail biting (tb) in pigs
  2. Terminology, specifying the various concepts involved in fp/tb.
  3. Overview of main similarities and differences between feather pecking and tail biting
  4. Farmer as a risk factor, emphasising, perhaps for the first time, that the farmer is a kind of ‘animal’ that is part of the problem (and the solution).
  5. Models, reviewing available conceptual models of fp and tb, as well as presenting a new ‘face model’.
  6. Disease framework, arguing that fp/tb may be regarded as a medical disorder, over and above being an abnormal behaviour per se.
  7. Evolution and domestication, emphasising the need to view fp/tb as a phenomenon an evolutionary and genetic background.
  8. References

The entire text (8 posts) can be downloaded as one pdf here.

4. Farmer as a risk factor

Though not specified in Brunberg et al. (2016) human stakeholders, i.c. the farmer, may also be included in the description of feather pecking (fp)/tail biting (tb). Since the farmer must make management decisions (at many different levels), he/she will affect all other risk factors involved in fp/tb. In fact, it has been proposed that the farmer may be the most important risk factor (Van Dooren, 2013; Zonderland and Zonderland-Thomassen, 2016). Like the animals, farmers will be showing behavioural responses, and farmers may also experience welfare problems in case of fp/tb.

As is the case for the animal, what matters to the farmer is how (s)he perceives the problem (rather than what is the problem in ‘reality’ (Uexküll, 1909)). For example, laying hens living in a multilayer system (volière) may be perceived as having access to litter. However, what matters to welfare is how the animal perceives its environment, e.g. a hen living in the upper tiers may not have access to litter, thus live in an environment without litter (and thus be more prone to fp). Similarly, a farmer who believes that beak treatment/tail docking is painless (a false belief), or who has a strong aversion to pecking/biting wounds may not have the motivation required to (try to) end the practice of routine preventive mutilations. Thus what matters is how the problem is perceived. The farmer’s problem solving in case of fp/tb may be hampered by being ‘allergic’ (e.g. to wounded animals due) to fp/tb, and by being ‘lethargic’ (e.g. being unable to respond adequately when active treatment is called for when fp/tb starts). While the farmer has the end responsibility of how his animals are reared, other stakeholders also play a role, e.g. a farmer’s ability to deal with fp/tb may depend on other farmers (e.g. who are rearing his animals, or who ventilate opinions as to whether ending mutilations is (not) desirable), the market (e.g. the retailer who is buying his eggs/pigs), the government (issuing legislation and taking policy measures to simulate and/or discourage certain practices), and farm advisors (e.g. providing/withholding knowledge and support required to deal with the fp/tb problem). Table 2 lists main farmer-related risk factors and responses involved in fp/tb. Note: we have decided to incorporate the responses in the list of risk factors, because the way a farmer responds to fp/tb (prevention, treatment, early-detection) is itself part of the farm management.

Table 2. Farmer-related risk factors of feather pecking (fp) and tail biting (tb), and farmer-related responses related to the type of factor (environment-based; farmer-based, and response-based (1; curative treatment; 2: prevention; 3. early detection). Red: welfare reducing aspects.

Farmer-related risk factors (management) & responses (prevention & treatment)Type of factor
Economy, marketEnvironment-based
Legislation (and its enforcement), policies 
Social support/pressure, sector/chain image 
Fp/tb specific farm management regarding (timely/delayed) treatment:
e.g. providing enrichment, identifying & resolving cause(s)/risk factor(s), isolation/removing actor and/or victim, dimming the lights (impaired vision), repellents, mutilation (as a treatment in untreated animals), monitoring (i.e. treatment evaluation).
Pigs: teeth cutting (cf beak treatment in Hens: removing animal’s ‘equipment’ to do harm).
Response [1]
Hens: spectacles (another form of impaired vision used in the past (before beak trimming was invented, to prevent accurate sight of feathers); culling of peckers (peckers may be identified by their pecking behaviour or perhaps by their relatively unaffected feather cover). 
Fp/tb specific farm management regarding prevention:
e.g. mutilation, enrichment, climate, food, health care,
monitoring (early detection)
Response [2]
Fp/tb specific farm management regarding early detection:
Hens: Reducing feather cover may indicate fp is starting
Pigs: Tucked tails, wet tails (possibly reduced feeder visits; enhanced interest in enrichment materials)
Response [3]
General farm management (quality; quantity)Farmer-based
Knowledge, education 
Personality (reactive, proactive) 
Attitudes (towards animal welfare, etc.). 

Reading guide

This was blog post nr. 4 under the heading of: “Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions”. It contains the following sections/posts:

  1. Introduction, specifying the need to compare feather pecking (fp) in layers and tail biting (tb) in pigs
  2. Terminology, specifying the various concepts involved in fp/tb.
  3. Overview of main similarities and differences between feather pecking and tail biting
  4. Farmer as a risk factor, emphasising, perhaps for the first time, that the farmer is a kind of ‘animal’ that is part of the problem (and the solution).
  5. Models, reviewing available conceptual models of fp and tb, as well as presenting a new ‘face model’.
  6. Disease framework, arguing that fp/tb may be regarded as a medical disorder, over and above being an abnormal behaviour per se.
  7. Evolution and domestication, emphasising the need to view fp/tb as a phenomenon an evolutionary and genetic background.
  8. References

The entire text (8 posts) can be downloaded as one pdf here.

Acknowledgements

These blog posts have been made possible by the Hennovation project (HORIZON 2020 ISIB-02-2014 project, Grant no. 652638).

Posted in Feather pecking, Laying hens | Tagged , , , , | Leave a comment

Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – 5. Models

This is post 5 on “Models” of:

Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions

Marc B.M. Brackea, T. Bas Rodenburgb, Herman M. Vermeera, Thea G.C.M. van Niekerka
a Wageningen Livestock Research
b Wageningen University, Dept. of behavioural ecology

Reading guide

This is one of 8 blog posts under the heading of: “Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions”. It contains the following sections/posts:

  1. Introduction, specifying the need to compare feather pecking (fp) in layers and tail biting (tb) in pigs
  2. Terminology, specifying the various concepts involved in fp/tb.
  3. Overview of main similarities and differences between feather pecking and tail biting
  4. Farmer as a risk factor, emphasising, perhaps for the first time, that the farmer is a kind of ‘animal’ that is part of the problem (and the solution).
  5. Models, reviewing available conceptual models of fp and tb, as well as presenting a new ‘face model’.
  6. Disease framework, arguing that fp/tb may be regarded as a medical disorder, over and above being an abnormal behaviour per se.
  7. Evolution and domestication, emphasising the need to view fp/tb as a phenomenon an evolutionary and genetic background.
  8. References

The entire text (8 posts) can be downloaded as one pdf here.

5. Models

Table 3 shows a list of various models/figures that have been proposed to clarify feather pecking (fp)/tail biting (tb), including the recent model proposed by Brunberg et al. (2016). Our focus here was to compare models, esp. models originating from Wageningen University Research, in search for potential improvements. Not all models have been included in Table 3. For example, Valros and Heinonen (2015) propose a modified bucket model where the bucket is filled with acute and/or chronic stressors (cf also Valros (2017)).

Some model illustrations.

Since copy-rights are a problem for representing models, below a selection is given of models for which Wageningen UR (already) has the copy-rights. Other models can be obtained via the cited references or the internet. E.g. an example of the psychohydraulic model (Lorenz, 1950; 1978) can be found here.

Figure 7.3 below shows the tail biting (tb) model by Zonderland (2010a) (Fig. 7.3, p. 138).

The conceptual framework for tb originating from Bracke (2008) (reprinted in (Bracke, 2017)) is shown in Figure 1 below. This model was designed to construct the RICHPIG model (decision support system) to assess/calculate the welfare value of enrichment materials for pigs.

Figure 1. Schematic representation of the conceptual framework for assessing environmental enrichment for pigs. EMat: Enrichment material; AMI: animal-material interactions; I: Istwert, the environment as perceived by the animal; S: Sollwert, set point or norm (modified homeostatic model after Wiepkema (1987) and (Anonymous, 2001)). (Figure from Bracke (2008), permission granted by UFAW) (reprinted from (Bracke, 2017)).

Citation from Bracke (2017) relating the model to the principle of communicating vessels:

Progressive feedback loops in the framework indicate that the animal’s welfare is good when proper enrichment satisfies the pigs’ need to explore and forage. When the enrichment is deficient, the animals will redirect their attention and show pen- and pen-mate directed behaviour. Note that this may imply a mechanism resembling the principle of communicating vessels (connected containers filled with liquid; see Wikipedia (2016c)). In accordance with this principle pigs may distribute their (motivation for) exploratory behaviour (the liquid) depending on the quality of the manipulable ‘materials’ available to them (cf Bracke et al. (2012)). Eventually, an outbreak of tail biting may occur, potentially evoking a positive feedback loop (an escalating outbreak) leading to cannibalism when no ‘proper enrichment’ is provided buffering and/or eliminating the (primary) cause/stressor.” (End of citation).

In the communicating-vessels model, for which we found some empirical evidence in pigs (Bracke, 2017), vessel size may change due to animal-properties like genetics; but also e.g. enrichment-based and other risk factors.

In the case of fp in poultry, in a classic paper Newberry et al. (2007) questioned the assumption of communicating vessels underlying the hypothesis that fp is redirected foraging behaviour as proposed earlier by Blokhuis (1986). Newberry et al. (2007) showed that birds with high levels of ground pecking as chicks were more likely to develop high levels of fp as adults compared to low ground pecking chicks. However, the high ground pecking chicks also continued to show high levels of ground pecking as adults, shedding doubt on the theory that fp would replace ground pecking.

Under ‘mechanism’ Van Niekerk (2015) presents both a balance model and a tipping-bucket model for fp (see also Van Niekerk (In prep.)). The bucket model was modified from a tb model originally proposed by Vermeer in Bracke et al. (2012). The main problem of the tipping-bucket model is that it suggests that fp/tb cannot stop, cannot be made undone (or perhaps only via an external ‘force’, e.g. a farmer taking adequate measures to correct the problem). Perhaps the model could be improved, e.g. by making a tumbler-type tipping bucket, such that it can be emptied, and then may restore its original position. However, this revised tumbler model would still be deficient in that post fp/tb set points are not the same as before (as a tumbler would suggest). Another option might be a series of buckets. Once tipped, the next bucket could stay down, with the next bucket being smaller, such that the next tipping point would be reached sooner, with preventive measures reducing the flow of water into the bucket. This would solve the issues just mentioned, but it would seem to be a somewhat ‘artificial’/non-parsimonious model.

Tipping bucket model of feather pecking

Figure 2. Tipping-bucket model of feather pecking (Van Niekerk (2015); modified after Bracke et al. (2012)).

Balance of causes of feather pecking

Figure 3. Balance model (Van Niekerk (2015), from http://www.henhub.eu/fp/mech/).

Perhaps the balance model could be modified to a balance between ‘fixed’ risk factors on the one scale and management (farmer effort) to reduce tb/fp risk on the other scale of the balance. However, the symmetry in disbalance suggested by the model does not seem to make sense: too much pressure on one side does not have the same effect as too much pressure on the other side. Also, fp/tb does not seem to be (totally) reversible: inducing fp/tb by removing a bit of enrichment cannot be undone by adding the same bit of enrichment (at least not shown). Also, to date no studies are available showing reversibility by adding other factors (e.g. inducing tb/fp by poor litter quality and then ‘treating’ this problem by adding e.g. better feed, etc.).

The next figure (Figure 4 below) shows a newly developed ‘face’ model aimed at incorporating the different types of animal involved (actor, victim, neutral), as well as emphasising the role of the farmer (as a kind of ‘actor’) in dealing with a fp/tb problem. The farmer is important for prevention and treatment of fp/tb. The emergence of an animal-actor is necessary to start fp/tb, but the responsiveness of the victim also plays a roll. For example, a victim may more or less effectively avoid becoming a victim and respond more or less in a way that leads to escalation of an outbreak. While a learning process may have transformed actors into individuals predisposed to show the abnormal fp/tb behaviour again at a later stage, similarly, at some point victims may show learned helplessness (which may also more or less permanently alter their behavioural predisposition).

Figure 4. New ‘face’ model of feather pecking (fp)/tail biting (tb), showing its multifactorial nature (‘left ear’), the role of different types of animal (actor & victim (‘eyes’), neutral (‘mouth’)), array of responses (‘right ear’), as well as the role of the farmer (‘nose’) in dealing with the problem. Both positive and negative feedback loops (‘glasses’ around the eyes of the face) are involved. Evolution and life history (‘hairs’) determine the set points of the individuals (animals and farmer). The comparators (‘pupils’ etc.) are (more or less) equivalent to welfare (smiley, balance, bucket and marble run) as indicated in the ‘necklace’ below the face. TIM: tail in mouth; OCTB: obsessive-compulsive tail biting; p.m.: pen mate; i.r.t.: in relation to. (Modified after (Bracke, 2017), and incorporating elements of the other models shown above, i.c. the balance and bucket models).

Reading guide

This was blog post nr. 5 under the heading of: “Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions”. It contains the following sections/posts:

  1. Introduction, specifying the need to compare feather pecking (fp) in layers and tail biting (tb) in pigs
  2. Terminology, specifying the various concepts involved in fp/tb.
  3. Overview of main similarities and differences between feather pecking and tail biting
  4. Farmer as a risk factor, emphasising, perhaps for the first time, that the farmer is a kind of ‘animal’ that is part of the problem (and the solution).
  5. Models, reviewing available conceptual models of fp and tb, as well as presenting a new ‘face model’.
  6. Disease framework, arguing that fp/tb may be regarded as a medical disorder, over and above being an abnormal behaviour per se.
  7. Evolution and domestication, emphasising the need to view fp/tb as a phenomenon an evolutionary and genetic background.
  8. References

The entire text (8 posts) can be downloaded as one pdf here.

Acknowledgements

These blog posts have been made possible by the Hennovation project (HORIZON 2020 ISIB-02-2014 project, Grant no. 652638).

Posted in Enrichment, Feather pecking, Laying hens | Tagged , , , , | Leave a comment

Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – 6. Disease framework

This is post 6. “Disease framework” of:

Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions

Marc B.M. Brackea, T. Bas Rodenburgb, Herman M. Vermeera, Thea G.C.M. van Niekerka
a Wageningen Livestock Research
b Wageningen University, Dept. of behavioural ecology

Reading guide

This is one of 8 blog posts under the heading of: “Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions”. It contains the following sections/posts:

  1. Introduction, specifying the need to compare feather pecking (fp) in layers and tail biting (tb) in pigs
  2. Terminology, specifying the various concepts involved in fp/tb.
  3. Overview of main similarities and differences between feather pecking and tail biting
  4. Farmer as a risk factor, emphasising, perhaps for the first time, that the farmer is a kind of ‘animal’ that is part of the problem (and the solution).
  5. Models, reviewing available conceptual models of fp and tb, as well as presenting a new ‘face model’.
  6. Disease framework, arguing that fp/tb may be regarded as a medical disorder, over and above being an abnormal behaviour per se.
  7. Evolution and domestication, emphasising the need to view fp/tb as a phenomenon an evolutionary and genetic background.
  8. References

The entire text (8 posts) can be downloaded as one pdf here.

6. Disease framework

Brunberg et al. (2016) characterise feather pecking (fp)/tail biting (tb) as an abnormal behaviour. However, they do not frame it as a disease. This section deals with the question whether fp/tb may/should be regarded as a disease, i.e. as a medical disorder, in particular a mental/behavioural pathology.

Of course, a lesion of the bitten/pecked animal (victim) can be regarded as a health disorder. However, when fp/tb is primarily seen as a behaviour of the actor, and not as a medical disorder, then adequately understanding the behaviour requires addressing the 4 why questions proposed by Tinbergen (1963). That is, a behaviour is sufficiently understood if we understand its mechanism/causation, its function/adaptation, its evolution/phylogeny (over generations) and its development/ontogeny (over the course of the individual’s life).

These aspects have been covered for the most part by Brunberg et al. (2016). However, aspects related to the importance and treatment of fp/tb were only marginally addressed.

When we were looking for a format to present the available information about fp at the www.henhub.eu/fp website, which was intended to inform farmers and the public, we decided to use the framework commonly used to describe medical disorders (Van Niekerk, 2015). This implies a characterisation of aspects like signs/symptoms/diagnosis (kinds of fp/tb), pathophysiology (mechanism), prevention, treatment and (economic) importance of fp.

Aspects related to (differential) diagnosis and pathophysiology may, for example, deal with the question whether ear biting, ear necrosis and flank biting are to be regarded as forms of (i.e. having a causation (and treatment) similar to) ‘tail biting. Conversely, Taylor et al. (2010) identified three types of tb in pigs: two-stage, sudden forceful and fanatic. These may be 3 different syndromes, all labelled ‘tb’.

The importance of the fp/tb problem does not only concern production losses and financial implications. It may also concern legal, psychological and ethical aspects such as animal integrity, the attitude regarding ‘blood’ in the pen, aversion to sustainability pressures among farmers, e.g. due to fear of (over-)regulation. As to the legal relevance, both tail docking and beak trimming have been banned in the EU some time ago, yet despite this farmers have mostly continued to perform these procedures routinely (and they were repeatedly being granted exemption to do so).

Once fp/tb is regarded as a problem that needs to be solved, the disease framework clearly has added value. For example, it is important to realise/understand that the management of risk factors applied for the purpose of prevention of fp/tb is not necessarily the same as applying them for the purpose of curative treatment, i.e. counteracting an outbreak of fp/tb. E.g. it is likely that more enrichment is needed to treat than to prevent fp/tb.

In the commercial practice of intensive livestock farming fp/tb are common problems, esp. when routine mutilations were no longer allowed. However, that does not imply that it is normal for the animals involved to show such abnormal behaviours. Fp/tb have been labelled so-called technopathies, i.e. pathological behaviours associated with agro-technologically designed living conditions. A common perception among applied ethologists, namely, is that there is a wide discrepancy between the animals’ living conditions in intensive livestock farming and the animals’ environment of evolutionary adaptation. That is, the living conditions are not normal. They are likely to overtax the animals’ control systems, thus leading to behavioural pathology/disorders. From an evolutionary perspective fp/tb is not part of the adaptive behaviours in which poultry/pigs deal with a variable environment. Instead these abnormal and injurious behaviours result from a frustrated need to perform rather species-specific foraging behaviours (scratching/rooting) for substantial periods of time, leading to boredom in relatively barren environments imposed under intensive livestock conditions.

The fact that these mutilating, harmful social behaviours (fp/tb) also (seem to) require preventive surgical interventions/mutilations (beak treatment, tail docking) strongly indicates that the label ‘medical disorder’ would seem to be appropriate. In humans self-harm and harmful social behaviour have been classified as (symptoms of) medical disorders, e.g. self-mutilation  in borderline patients and antisocial/dissocial personality disorder respectively. Preventive mutilation also implies an infringement of the animals’ integrity (a moral concern). It also indicates that the environment is not suited for the animals. Examples of preventive mutilations in human medicine are (religious) circumcision (e.g. for religious reasons or to prevent HIV in sub-Saharan Africa (Siegfried et al., 2009)), preventive mastectomy in women who are BRCA1 or BRCA2 mutation gene carriers (Zagouri et al., 2013), and prophylactic colectomy or even Whipple procedure (a major surgical operation involving the removal of the head of the pancreas, the duodenum, the proximal jejunum, gallbladder, and part of the stomach), e.g. in case of familial adenomatous polyposis (i.e. to prevent intestinal cancer). Whether in humans or animals, preventive surgery, even if intended to prevent ‘greater harm’ later in life, is not to be regarded as ‘normal’. E.g. in the case of pigs and laying hens the preventive mutilations have been associated with chronic ‘phantom’ pain because of neuroma formation (pigs: (Simonsen et al., 1991; Di Giminiani et al., 2017); poultry: (Gentle, 1986)). And, while the mutilation may solve the problem of future victims, in doing so it masks the problem (stress, mental pathology) that gives rise to neutrals becoming actors that engage in destructive, harmful social (or sometimes self-mutilating (van Zeeland et al., 2009)) behaviours. To paraphrase an early Dutch researcher of tail biting in pigs, Gerrit van Putten: the pig’s tail is a thermometer of animal welfare, which was discarded when the “temperature” became too high, i.e. the tails were docked rather than that housing conditions were improved. Also regarding the practice of preventing fp/tb using routine mutilations, it has been pointed out that these procedures are not fully effective. E.g. Zonderland et al. (2011) estimated a prevalence of 2.12% tail biting despite (very short) tail docking on Dutch farms.

Another indicator for viewing fp/tb as a medical disorder is the fact that fp/tb outbreaks lead to injuries (wounds), which may even escalate into cannibalism and/or death due to secondary infections of the wounds. This (progressive) loss of normal functioning and lack of homeostasis have typically been regarded as indicative of disease, esp. since these are also of economic significance to the farmers (Zonderland et al., 2011).

However, fp/tb is not just a problem because of wounds inflicted on the victims. As Brunberg et al. (2016) rightly point out, the (acute pain of the) victim is not the only welfare concern. An important part of the (more chronic) welfare problem concerns the stressors/behavioural deprivations that lead neutral animals to become actors of fp/tb. Fp/tb, therefore, is not just a medial disorder because of the clinical wounds (and surgical prevention practices), but also because of the likely mental disorder leading to the abnormal fp/tb behaviour shown by the actors. Taylor et al. (2010) distinguished three types of tail-biting pigs: two-stage (where tail biting is preceded by more gentle tail-in-mouth (TIM) behaviour), sudden forceful (without prior TIM, e.g. to get access to the feeder) and fanatic tb. The latter was also labelled as ‘obsessive’ and ‘persistent’, and may thus be classified tentatively as an ‘obsessive-compulsive’, which seems to come close to labelling this type of tb as an obsessive-compulsive mental disorder.

Formerly, harmful social behaviours like fp and tb have been labelled as ‘vices’. However, that label implies seeing the actors as a kind of criminals. In fact, and esp. the medical framework, would turn the actor into a kind of victim too, i.e. a victim of inappropriate/depriving/stressful living conditions of intensive farming.

A related point indicative of fp/tb being a medical disorder is the fact that fp/tb problems tend to spread in a pen (and perhaps also across pens). This may make fp/tb resemble an infectious disease/epidemic. In other words, fp/tb has disease-like properties: the behaviour has a tendency to escalate into a fp/tb outbreak. While it may be that novelty and reward (the taste of blood) may account for the frantic and ‘contagious’ appearance, the behaviour seems to be ‘contagious’, in that other animals in a pen/enclosure may acquire the behaviour once the first animal has started to engage in it. In this respect, it may be noted, that esp. in pigs often the pen is regarded as an ‘experimental unit’, while it remains to be shown that pigs in neighbouring pens remain unaffected by (the arousal caused by) ongoing tb.

A further disease-like property of fp/tb is the role of stress in the aetiology of the problem.  Many diseases are aggravated by common stressors like high stocking densities and limited access of food or a poor climate, e.g. because stress may reduce the immune response needed to combat the disease. Similar stressors also play a role in triggering fp/tb. However, the underlying pathophysiological mechanism may not be the same, as stimulation of the (humoral) immune response has been shown to predispose (rather than inhibit) fp in laying hens (Parmentier et al., 2009).

Finally, it may be pointed out that most major textbooks describing the diseases of poultry/laying hens and swine/pigs already have a chapter or section on fp and tb respectively. In that respect, a start has been made to recognise fp and tb as a medical/mental/behavioural pathology. The conclusion of this section, we believe, justifies a more proper recognition of fp/tb as a medical disorder, i.c. a mental disorder, in the future.

Reading guide

This was blog post nr. 6 under the heading of: “Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions”. It contains the following sections/posts:

  1. Introduction, specifying the need to compare feather pecking (fp) in layers and tail biting (tb) in pigs
  2. Terminology, specifying the various concepts involved in fp/tb.
  3. Overview of main similarities and differences between feather pecking and tail biting
  4. Farmer as a risk factor, emphasising, perhaps for the first time, that the farmer is a kind of ‘animal’ that is part of the problem (and the solution).
  5. Models, reviewing available conceptual models of fp and tb, as well as presenting a new ‘face model’.
  6. Disease framework, arguing that fp/tb may be regarded as a medical disorder, over and above being an abnormal behaviour per se.
  7. Evolution and domestication, emphasising the need to view fp/tb as a phenomenon an evolutionary and genetic background.
  8. References

The entire text (8 posts) can be downloaded as one pdf here.

Acknowledgements

These blog posts have been made possible by the Hennovation project (HORIZON 2020 ISIB-02-2014 project, Grant no. 652638).

Posted in Feather pecking, Laying hens | Tagged , , , | Leave a comment

Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – 7. Evolution and domestication

This is post 7 on “Evolution and Domestication” of:

Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions

Marc B.M. Brackea, T. Bas Rodenburgb, Herman M. Vermeera, Thea G.C.M. van Niekerka
a Wageningen Livestock Research
b Wageningen University, Dept. of behavioural ecology

Reading guide

This is one of 8 blog posts under the heading of: “Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions”. It contains the following sections/posts:

  1. Introduction, specifying the need to compare feather pecking (fp) in layers and tail biting (tb) in pigs
  2. Terminology, specifying the various concepts involved in fp/tb.
  3. Overview of main similarities and differences between feather pecking and tail biting
  4. Farmer as a risk factor, emphasising, perhaps for the first time, that the farmer is a kind of ‘animal’ that is part of the problem (and the solution).
  5. Models, reviewing available conceptual models of fp and tb, as well as presenting a new ‘face model’.
  6. Disease framework, arguing that fp/tb may be regarded as a medical disorder, over and above being an abnormal behaviour per se.
  7. Evolution and domestication, emphasising the need to view fp/tb as a phenomenon an evolutionary and genetic background.
  8. References

The entire text (8 posts) can be downloaded as one pdf here.

7 Evolution & domestication

This final post/section aims to emphasise that adopting a disease framework for feather pecking (fp)/tail biting (tb) does not imply discarding the common science-based and evolutionary perspective on fp/tb. In order to show why this may be important we will first consider non-scientific reasoning to deal with fp/tb.

From a non-scientific and non-welfare perspective it may make perfect sense to farmers and veterinarians to prevent or treat fp/tb using respectively beak treatment (i.e. removing the means for fp) and tail docking (i.e. removing the object of tb). Similarly, measures like spectacles to prevent accurate vision (preventive measure in poultry) and teeth cutting (treatment measure in pigs) have been used, as has been the keeping of animals in the dark (thus blocking the animals’ vision). Along these lines one may also propose breeding poultry without feathers and pigs without tails, hens with blunted beaks, and pigs without incisor teeth, or perhaps blind animals (Ali and Cheng, 1985) (e.g. without eyes). Similarly, physical restrictions may be imposed in theory, e.g. solitary confinement/individual housing would be highly effective in stopping fp/tb. A related ‘solution’ is a limited physical ability to move (rather than lack of motivation (Bokkers and Koene, 2004)), as appears to be the case in heavily selected broilers. In fact, this may (partly) explain why fp is much less of a problem in broilers compared to laying hens. When comparing broilers to pigs, another reason, besides the limited physical activity, may be age. Broilers are slaughtered at 5-6 weeks of age, while egg-laying (puberty) starts at around 17 weeks (when severe fp normally develops). In pigs slaughter age and puberty are around 6 months and tb may be seen roughly in the period between 4 weeks and 6 months. Perhaps the situation in pigs, where tb is frequently seen in weaned and young growing pigs, and in rearing gilts but not in pregnant/farrowing sows, is somewhat more comparable to turkeys, where fp is a problem (5-8% in untreated turkeys; 10-16% in beaktreated turkeys) at around 4 days of age and around 8-10 weeks of age, at which age egg-laying/puberty may also start, while slaughter age is around 16-20 weeks (Van Niekerk and Bracke, 2016; van Niekerk and Veldkamp, 2017). Turkeys, like pigs, have been bred less intensively for muscle growth compared to broilers (Van Niekerk and Bracke, 2016). However, it may also be noted that fp in turkeys does not seem to respond as favourably to enrichment as does fp in laying hens (Van Niekerk and Bracke, 2016) and tb in pigs. In line with these considerations, the comparison between fp in poultry (laying hens and broilers) and tb in weaned/growing pigs, would raise the tentative suggestion that while fp is less prevalent in fast-growing broilers because of their very young age and limited physical activity, slower-growing broilers, in virtue of the older age and enhanced physical activity, should be expected to have an enhanced propensity to show fp behaviour. An anonymous poultry-welfare expert (pers. comm.) indicates that this may indeed be the case.

A risk of using breeding for inactivity to reduce fp/tb, of course, could be that in addition to reducing the propensity of the actor to show harmful social behaviour, inactivity may also reduce the propensity of the victim to avoid being pecked/bitten. Another breeding goal may thus be to select for animals that do not have the (cognitive) capacity to ‘discover’ fp/tb, and/or to breed against the ability to acquire the behaviour through social transmission (i.e. to learn from conspecifics who have become actors). Such selection for ‘stupidity’ is also unlikely to be effective, because both laying hens and pigs need a certain level of cognitive functioning and synchronisation, e.g. to regulate access to limited resources like nest boxes and feeders (Boumans, 2017).

A major factor in causing fp/tb is the animals’ motivation to explore/forage. Would it then make sense to select against this motivation per se?

In applied ethology it is commonly assumed that fp/tb are caused or at least mediated by a deprived motivation to forage. The idea is that poultry and pigs still have behavioural needs originating from evolution in a natural environment. Domestication is not perceived to have had a major attenuating effect, i.e. modern pigs and poultry are not (yet) adapted to intensive farming. The motivation to forage is still considerable because it was essential to survive in a natural environment spending considerable periods of time searching for food. Being generalist omnivores also implied these animals had relatively inquisitive natures to investigate a wide variety of potential food items under variable circumstances encountered in nature.

In fact, this attraction to novelty and eagerness to learn may well be sufficient to explain one of the most characteristic features of fp/tb, namely that a kind of irreversible state change occurs once the first fp/tb has taken place, and also that the problem has a certain tendency to escalate and is much more difficult to counteract later than it is to prevent it from occurring in the first place. A normal learning process can thus explain the difference in set point between animals who have never experienced fp/tb and those that have. No pathology need to be involved here.

Esp. pigs that are provided with novel enrichment materials clearly show ‘fanatic’, almost compulsive behaviours, except that the behavioural intensity tends to wear off readily (it is mostly a matter of a few quarters rather than hours that pigs spend on interacting with new enrichment materials). However, when the enrichment is slowly destructible (like soft wood), designed to fit the needs of the animal (e.g. branched chain design, (Bracke, 2017)) or provides (irregular) food rewards, e.g. as in the case of the Edinburgh foodball in pigs (releasing food pellets upon being rooted and thus moved around the pen (Young et al., 1994)), much more persistent (and less fanatic) interest may be observed. Pigs and poultry also clearly appreciate the taste of blood and (tail/feather/skin) tissue.

Note also that in addition to being potentially explained as a cognitive (learning) process, the escalation of fp/tb and (subsequent) state change may also be related to cognition and a tendency to show synchronised (feeding/exploration/activity) behaviour. A related potentially-involved mechanism could be the supposedly powerful tendency to show conformism, as suggested by De Waal in the case of primates (De Waal, 2016).

E.g. van de Waal et al. (2013) showed that green monkeys that had been trained to prefer maize of one colour, would unlearn their previous colour preference and acquire the colour preference of the group they had been introduced into. Similarly, mixing a less friendly primate species with a more friendly species, made the former much (4 times) more friendly (De Waal and Johanowicz, 1993). Uitdehaag et al. (2009) found that mixed housing of a more and less fearful strain of laying hens negatively affected fp and fear-related behaviour. Perhaps conformism may play a role in fp/tb in that once more and more individuals start to show the behaviour, other individuals may have a strong tendency to do the same. Thus conformism may explain (part of the escalation) by potentiation, but it cannot explain its origin (though it may explain why there is a reluctance to show fp/tb in a group that has never experienced it before). (Note: the origin may also be more or less accidental, e.g. McAdie and Keeling (2000) showed that (artificially) damaged feathers may trigger (outbreaks of) fp in laying hens.)

Counteracting the motivation to show fp/tb by genetic selection may simultaneously counteract the animals’ motivation to consume feed and thus (efficiently) produce under commercial conditions. Pigs and poultry need to be eager to consume food and they must readily accept novel feeds (e.g. when moved from rearing farm to finishing/egg-laying farm).

Thus, the motivation to forage may not only be a remnant of evolution in a natural environment, it may also be a product of selection for maximised production efficiency. In other words, domestication and genetic selection may have been co-shaping the current problem underlying fp/tb in intensive pig and poultry farming.

Traditionally, pigs and poultry have been selected using individual selection, i.e. the fastest growing individuals were selected to breed the next generation, perhaps even when individuals were showing high levels of production at the expense of pen mates (e.g. due to excessive aggression or the performance of fp/tb). In particular, when fp/tb occurred the (most heavily affected) victims were unlikely to be used for reproduction, but the actors in a pen could partly go unnoticed (unless they were detected and eliminated from the group). Group selection has been proposed as an alternative to individual selection, where the production efficiency of pen mates is also taken to load on an individual’s selection potential (Muir, 2003; Bijma et al., 2007a; Bijma et al., 2007b). Group selection has thus been suggested as a potential solution for fp/tb by selecting for peaceful pigs/poultry. Such peaceful pigs, however, may be less motivated to forage, and thus be less efficient for production.

Genetic selection probably has made use of the evolutionary tendency of animals (esp. males) to grow fast so as to have a higher likelihood of reproduction (as the largest individuals of a generation tend to win fights for access to females). Fast growth (as required for pigs and broilers), however, requires a persistent appetite to sustain growth. Compared to egg-laying in hens, which similarly require a substantial appetite to be able to sustain a high egg production, the biological prioritisation is different. Resource allocation theory suggests that animals make adaptive adjustments in the allocation of resources to different life processes when facing changed selection pressures (Beilharz et al., 1993). Hens must prioritise allocating energy to their offspring (eggs), whereas pigs and broilers must (i.e. have been selected to) allocate energy to their own growth (so as to produce meat). Thus, both types of farm animals are also likely to have been selected to prioritise (as much as possible) those processes that are preferred by man (be it the production of meat or eggs). Thus, when dealing with (mild) disease states it is possible that farm animals have been selected to (tend to) prioritise production over the (energetically costly) activation of the immune response. Farm animals that continue ‘functioning’ in an economic/zootechnical sense, however, may not be the most productive overall, e.g. when enhanced appetite has a negative side-effect in increasing the likelihood of fp/tb.

Several observations may be in line with the suggestion that in modern farm animals appetitive foraging motivation may have originated in a discrepancy with the natural environment, but may also have been co-determined by genetic selection for maximised production efficiency. A main indicator is that adult animals, esp. pregnant sows and broiler breeders, are known to experience high levels of feeding motivation and a tendency to become obese when given  ad lib access to feed. (In laying hens, however, fp rather seems to be associated with hyper-mobility, also called a hyperactivity disorder (Kjaer, 2009), perhaps related to an (over-)activated foraging motivation.) In addition, when growing pigs are feeding they seem to be focussed so much on feed intake that vaccinating them with a rather painful (large diameter) needle seems to go (largely) unnoticed. Also, growing pigs whose front teeth have been cut so as to counteract an outbreak of tail biting don’t seem to show a clear reduction in feed intake. When their teeth have been cut, however, the pigs are much less inclined to continue tail biting and they also show much less interest in manipulating enrichment materials like chains, wood and ropes, suggesting that they do feel pain while maintaining a relatively high motivation to feed.

A final aspect where a science-based evolutionary understanding of fp/tb behaviour has clear added value over a classic medical framework may be the observation that not all kinds of stressors are equally likely to contribute to fp/tb. This may be an important aspect of the pathophysiology/mechanism underlying fp/tb. In laying hens, for example, organic farmers say that pullets with access to an outdoor range are less fearful later in life (e.g. in using the outdoor range), and therefore are less likely to develop fp (TvN, pers. comm.). Also in laying hens, the stressor of being moved from the rearing farm to the layer facility appears to be a trigger of stress and fp (even though it may also be a ‘revival’ of fp that originated at the rearing farm). In pigs, by contrast, mixing is not typically eliciting tail biting, despite the fact that it is highly stressful for the pigs. E.g. Holinger (2017) found no effect of mixing and isolation stress on tail and ear manipulation in pigs. Note also that mixing in pigs typically occurs at 25kg body weight (10-12 weeks of age), which is long before puberty at slaughter age, i.e. about 5-6 months of age, whereas laying hens are transferred to the laying facilities shortly before egg-laying starts (i.e. puberty). In pigs this compares to the rearing of breeding gilts, who are also particularly prone to tb (probably because they are fed on more restricted diets than slaughter pigs are), but only at a younger age (i.e. before the gilts are inseminated). Tb in pigs is hardly seen in (first or older parity) pregnant sows and in this respect tb in pigs differs from fp in laying hens. Such differences must be kept in mind, but they should not overrule the striking similarities between fp in poultry and tb in pigs, nor should they be regarded as a counterargument to the proposition that fp/tb would certainly benefit from being regarded as a medical/mental health disorder, provided the existing science-based and evolutionary framework is maintained to understand the behavioural as well.

Reading guide

This was blog post nr. 7 under the heading of: “Towards a common conceptual framework and illustrative model for feather pecking in poultry and tail biting in pigs – Connecting science to solutions”. It contains the following sections/posts:

  1. Introduction, specifying the need to compare feather pecking (fp) in layers and tail biting (tb) in pigs
  2. Terminology, specifying the various concepts involved in fp/tb.
  3. Overview of main similarities and differences between feather pecking and tail biting
  4. Farmer as a risk factor, emphasising, perhaps for the first time, that the farmer is a kind of ‘animal’ that is part of the problem (and the solution).
  5. Models, reviewing available conceptual models of fp and tb, as well as presenting a new ‘face model’.
  6. Disease framework, arguing that fp/tb may be regarded as a medical disorder, over and above being an abnormal behaviour per se.
  7. Evolution and domestication, emphasising the need to view fp/tb as a phenomenon an evolutionary and genetic background.
  8. References

The entire text (8 posts) can be downloaded as one pdf here.

Acknowledgements

These blog posts have been made possible by the Hennovation project (HORIZON 2020 ISIB-02-2014 project, Grant no. 652638).

Posted in Enrichment, Feather pecking, Laying hens | Tagged , , , | Leave a comment